Moving Towards a More Inclusive Model of Reading: the Active View of Reading

There are many different models of reading in English. Each of these has been developed in an attempt to explain the very complex skill of reading. 

Earlier this year I was introduced to a model of reading that I am very excited about – The Active View of Reading – and that I want to share with you.  But before I do that, I want to talk about some of the different reading models out there, which will help to explain my excitement.


In English speaking countries, most of the models currently in use are based on the Simple View of Reading.  The Simple View was developed by Gough and Tunmer (1986) with the aim of ending the “reading wars”.  The term “reading wars” is used to describe when educators have opposing points of view about one area of literacy development being more important than another.  At the time that Gough and Tunmer published the Simple View of Reading, many literacy experts believed that either language or phonics was the most important area to target for a developing reader – and had heated debates about the topic.  However, the truth is that for each reader both factors are equally important, and so the Simple View of Reading was born and is now widely accepted.

Simple View Of Reading

Since 1986, a number of other models have been developed, using Simple View of Reading as a basis.  Each of these provides elaborations that we have greatly needed for literacy teaching and learning.


In 2000, I was first introduced to Cunningham’s Whole-to-Part (1993) model by the amazing David Koppenhaver and Karen Erickson.  In common with most of the other models based on the Simple View of Reading, Cunningham moved away from the term “decoding” and used the phrase “word identification” instead.  This is important since fluent readers tend to recognise many words automatically without needing to decode them – although decoding does still occur when required. Cunningham recognises this and refers to both “automatic word identification” and “mediated word identification” in his model.

Silent Reading With Comprehension

In 2001, Scarborough published the Reading Rope model which shows “the many strands that are woven into skilled reading”.  This is another model that I have used for some time and which I find extremely helpful as it breaks the two main areas of the Simple View of Reading down for us.  The Reading Rope model connects early language and literacy to later reading ability and disability and helps us think about all the components that we need to address in our literacy intervention.  

Rope Model W

One of the important parts of this model is Scarborough’s premise that all the components are interconnected and interdependent. She wants us to understand that if just one strand is weak, it affects the rope (and the reader) as a whole. And, of course, the visual presentation of this model showing all the subskills knitting together to lead to the goal of skilled reading, really helps us to understand this.


More recently, Tunmer & Hoover (2019) provided a model that they called the “Cognitive Foundations Framework”. Once again, this breaks the two main areas from the Simple View of Reading down into a variety of sub-areas.  As you can see, there are some similarities and differences between the Rope Model and the Cognitive Foundations of Reading model – and each of them gives us slightly different areas to consider for both our teaching and assessment.

The Cognitive Foundations Framework

One of the differences between the two models that I want to draw your attention to, is that in The Reading Rope, print concepts falls under Language Comprehension, but in the Cognitive Foundations Framework it falls under Word Recognition.  I’ll get back to this further on!

In developing the Cognitive Foundations Framework, the authors acknowledge that it doesn’t deal with factors outside of cognitive ones. This means it doesn’t include psychological and ecological factors. They also make it clear that this is a hierarchical structure.  Despite this, they don’t suggest that the “development of the higher-order cognitive elements cannot occur until all of the lower-order elements are fully developed – although it does assume that some level of mastery is needed.” (Tunmer & Hoover (2019).


And then early this year my friend Nick Drover (hi Nick), pointed me towards the Active View of Reading.  I got very excited as it offers us a strong progression, still based on the Simple View of Reading, but incorporating additional understandings about reading with comprehension.

Active View Of Reading

There are so many things I am excited about with this model!

As you can see, it again has different skills listed under the two areas from the Simple View of Reading.  Additionally, it has represented those two areas as circles that intersect.  The authors have called that intersection “Bridging Processes”.  Bridging Processes has given the authors a place to position skills, like print concepts, that often jump from area to area across other models as I mentioned earlier.  It also gives them a place to put fluency, which has a huge chunk of research devoted to it, but which often doesn’t appear in other models for a range of reasons. 

I am also really excited about the list of skills that fall under Language Comprehension. The authors give us a broad view of language that acknowledges factors like cultural knowledge that aren’t mentioned in other models – but still includes language structure and background knowledge.  And they bring in theory of mind – which has a massive impact on comprehension.  Hooray!

The Word Recognition section consists of the expected range of skills in this area. But, most importantly, they begin their model by including an extra area that is a critical component for reading with comprehension – active self-regulation.

Active Self-Regulation impacts students I work with every week.  Not just in reading, of course, but across their day.  If you are reading this blog, you are probably working on literacy with one or more students who cannot rely on speech alone to be heard and understood and/or who are neurodivergent – and I know this means that you also realise the impact this area has.  Having it acknowledged in this model is incredibly important for many students as well as for educators.  It is something we need to consider at all times – and every day we need to support students to learn to actively self-regulate by starting with co-regulation strategies and moving towards self-regulation where possible.

And then the authors delighted me even more, by publishing an evaluation of the components of their model and then discussing the implications for social justice.  Yay!  The diagram below shows the model with the effect sizes for each area. 

Active View Of Reading With Effect Sizes

If you want to read the original article about the Active View of Reading, the article that introduces it is available here.

I hope this model is as exciting to you as it is to me. I’d love to hear what you think of it in the comments.

(And if you would like to read Hoover and Tunmer’s discussion of the Active View, that is also freely available.)


References

Burns, M. K., Duke, N. K., & Cartwright, K. B. (2023). Evaluating components of the active view of reading as intervention targets: Implications for social justice, School Psychology, 38(1), 30–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000519

Cunningham, J.W. (1993). Whole‐To‐Part Reading Diagnosis, Reading & Writing Quarterly, 9(1), 31-49. DOI: 10.1080/1057356930090103

Duke, N.K., & Cartwright, K.B. (2021). The Science of Reading Progresses: Communicating Advances Beyond the Simple View of Reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(1), 25–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.411

Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, Reading, and Reading Disability, Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6-10. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258600700104

Hoover, W. A., & Tunmer, W. E. (2022). The Primacy of Science in Communicating Advances in the Science of Reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 57(2), 399–408. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.446

Scarborough, H. S. (2001).Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S.B Neuman & D.K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97-110). New York: Guilford Press.

Tunmer, W.E. & Hoover, W.A. (2019). The cognitive foundations of learning to read: a framework for preventing and remediating reading difficulties, Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 24:1, 75-93, DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2019.1614081

Spread the word. Share this post!

Comments (9)

  1. Victoria

    Reply

    I was wondering where does Science of Reading and Australian schools moving to that model of decoding (explicitly named in the curriculum now) fit in this context?

    • jane

      Reply

      HI Victoria, “Science of Reading” is just a name for the overall body of reading research that each of these models is based on – and the science of reading tells us that there are many subskills that students need to learn – as each of these models does. Australian Curriculum V9 makes it clear that we need to teach phonic knowledge (it doesn’t call it decoding as you’ll see if you do a search on the website), just as Australian Curriculum also specifies that we should use comprehension strategies with developing readers. So far, neither the curriculum nor a state has mandated an approach that needs to be used for either of these areas, nor for any other. We just need to keep reading the research, informing our practice and making sure we do the best for our students. Any of these models, including the Active View, definitely fits with that headset and with Australian Curriculum. Hope that helps!

  2. abamb0

    Reply

    Love that active self-regulation is included and fore fronted. Would love more discussion on this….What do the different effect sizes mean we should be doing/focussing on in reading instruction? When using the Comprehensive Literacy for ALL instructional practices, do we have enough focus on blending sounds and segmenting words?

    • jane

      Reply

      HI Anna, we’re constantly reviewing what we do as you know. The different effect sizes certainly provide a new lens for looking at literacy – although I still strongly believe that we need to teach all parts of literacy for all students to learn to read and write. Just because one part of any of the models can have a bigger effect for some students, each of these models shows us that there is really good basis for doing comprehensive literacy with all students. In terms of phonics, we absolutely need to get better at phonics instruction – I just think that we’re at a difficult point. The students we work with aren’t included in the phonics research referred to often by the phrase “science of reading”. The little bit of research we have suggests that our students might need different instruction to their peers – but we don’t have enough research to say that conclusively. The Autism specific literacy research suggests a large number of Autistic students find learning phonics much easier than learning comprehension – so we also need to make sure that we do that really well. Again, we could do with a lot more information than we currently have, but at least in this area the research we do have suggests that similar strategies the ones referred to in the research called “Science of Reading” do work with our students – we just need to be more explicit in teaching them, have more repetition and have smaller groups. I love that we are at a point that we can have these discussions – but I also wish we were another 10 or 20 years down the track and knew a lot more!! We just have to keep keeping up with the research and constantly improving our practice!

  3. Kylie Gough

    Reply

    thanks so much Jane, do you know of any training offered in relation to this model?

  4. Sonya Judson

    Reply

    Hi Jane,
    I am pleased to read your support of the Active View of Reading. I too, am glad to see the inclusion of active self regulation as a critical factor in students being best placed to receive and process new knowledge. The bridge from decoding and word recognition, to fluency is a big one and needs an explicit systematic system that is personalized for each student. The evidence is there though, within 3 – 6 months with my complex neurodivergent, amazing students. It is a true joy to see their engagement and progress as readers and writers.

Leave a comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *